Welcome to DU! The truly grassroots left-of-center political community where regular people, not algorithms, drive the discussions and set the standards. Join the community: Create a free account Support DU (and get rid of ads!): Become a Star Member Latest Breaking News General Discussion The DU Lounge All Forums Issue Forums Culture Forums Alliance Forums Region Forums Support Forums Help & Search

Panich52

(5,829 posts)
Thu Feb 26, 2015, 04:15 PM Feb 2015

Why Reading and Writing on Paper Beat Digital Screens For Boosting Your Brain

Why Reading and Writing on Paper Beat Digital Screens For Boosting Your Brain

Research shows that writing leads to an increase in conceptual understanding, application and retention.
By Tom Chatfield / The Guardian

My son is 18 months old, and I’ve been reading books with him since he was born. I say “reading”, but I really mean “looking at” – not to mention grasping, dropping, throwing, cuddling, chewing, and everything else a tiny human being likes to do. Over the last six months, though, he has begun not simply to look but also to recognise a few letters and numbers. He calls a capital Y a “yak” after a picture on the door of his room; a capital H is “hedgehog”; a capital K, “kangaroo”; and so on.

Reading, unlike speaking, is a young activity in evolutionary terms. Humans have been speaking in some form for hundreds of thousands of years; we are born with the ability to acquire speech etched into our neurones. The earliest writing, however, emerged only 6,000 years ago, and every act of reading remains a version of what my son is learning: identifying the special species of physical objects known as letters and words, using much the same neural circuits as we use to identify trees, cars, animals and telephone boxes.

It’s not only words and letters that we process as objects. Texts themselves, so far as our brains are concerned, are physical landscapes. So it shouldn’t be surprising that we respond differently to words printed on a page compared to words appearing on a screen; or that the key to understanding these differences lies in the geography of words in the world.

For her new book, Words Onscreen: The Fate of Reading in a Digital World, linguistics professor Naomi Baron conducted a survey of reading preferences among over 300 university students across the US, Japan, Slovakia and Germany. When given a choice between media ranging from printouts to smartphones, laptops, e-readers and desktops, 92% of respondents replied that it was hard copy that best allowed them to concentrate.

This isn’t a result likely to surprise many editors, or anyone else who works closely with text. While writing this article, I gathered my thoughts through a version of the same principle: having collated my notes onscreen, I printed said notes, scribbled all over the resulting printout, argued with myself in the margins, placed exclamation marks next to key points, spread out the scrawled result – and from this landscape hewed a (hopefully) coherent argument.

What exactly was going on here? Age and habit played their part. But there is also a growing scientific recognition that many of a screen’s unrivalled assets – search, boundless and bottomless capacity, links and leaps and seamless navigation – are either unhelpful or downright destructive when it comes to certain kinds of reading and writing.

Across three experiments in 2013, researchers Pam Mueller and Daniel Oppenheimer compared the effectiveness of students taking longhand notes versus typing onto laptops. Their conclusion: the relative slowness of writing by hand demands heavier “mental lifting”, forcing students to summarise rather than to quote verbatim – in turn tending to increase conceptual understanding, application and retention.

In other words, friction is good – at least so far as the remembering brain is concerned. Moreover, the textured variety of physical writing can itself be significant. In a 2012 study at Indiana University, psychologist Karin James tested five-year-old children who did not yet know how to read or write by asking them to reproduce a letter or shape in one of three ways: typed onto a computer, drawn onto a blank sheet, or traced over a dotted outline. When the children were drawing freehand, an MRI scan during the test showed activation across areas of the brain associated in adults with reading and writing. The other two methods showed no such activation.

Similar effects have been found in other tests, suggesting not only a close link between reading and writing, but that the experience of reading itself differs between letters learned through handwriting and letters learned through typing. Add to this the help that the physical geography of a printed page or the heft of a book can provide to memory, and you’ve got a conclusion neatly matching our embodied natures: the varied, demanding, motor-skill-activating physicality of objects tends to light up our brains brighter than the placeless, weightless scrolling of words on screens.

In many ways, this is an unfair result, effectively comparing print at its best to digital at its worst. Spreading my scrawled-upon printouts across a desk, I’m not just accessing data; I’m reviewing the idiosyncratic geography of something I created, carried and adorned. But I researched my piece online, I’m going to type it up onscreen, and my readers will enjoy an onscreen environment expressly designed to gift resonance: a geography, a context. Screens are at their worst when they ape and mourn paper. At their best, they’re something free to engage and activate our wondering minds in ways undreamt of a century ago.

Above all, it seems to me, we must abandon the notion that there is only one way of reading, or that technology and paper are engaged in some implacable war. We’re lucky enough to have both growing self-knowledge and an opportunity to make our options as fit for purpose as possible – as slippery and searchable or slow with friction as the occasion demands.

I can’t imagine teaching my son to read in a house without any physical books, pens or paper. But I can’t imagine denying him the limitless words and worlds a screen can bring to him either. I hope I can help him learn to make the most of both – and to type/copy/paste/sketch/scribble precisely as much as he needs to make each idea his own.

More info links in orig:
http://www.alternet.org/personal-health/why-reading-and-writing-paper-beat-digital-screens-boosting-your-brain?akid=12825.1924881.Vmf6UK&rd=1&src=newsletter1032384&t=23

••
I much prefer reading an actual book than something onscreen. (Tho on a screen is easier to copy -- like to post on DU. )

Back in the Dark Ages, when I was in college, I filled notebooks w/ the info I'd hilighted in my texts -- I'd discovered I remembered it better if I wrote it down. Now there's evidence I was right! Amazing!
••

3 replies = new reply since forum marked as read
Highlight: NoneDon't highlight anything 5 newestHighlight 5 most recent replies
Why Reading and Writing on Paper Beat Digital Screens For Boosting Your Brain (Original Post) Panich52 Feb 2015 OP
I knew this already. Manifestor_of_Light Feb 2015 #1
About ten or so years ago, SheilaT Feb 2015 #2
I approve of tablets/computers in schools, but as supplement, not replacement Panich52 Feb 2015 #3
 

Manifestor_of_Light

(21,046 posts)
1. I knew this already.
Thu Feb 26, 2015, 04:19 PM
Feb 2015

I'm auditory and kinesthetic.

Back in college, I knew I had to go to class and listen to the teacher (Auditory) and take notes (kinesthetic) or I couldn't learn the material. I didn't know why until I found out about the three modes of learning--auditory, kinesthetic and visual.

Most people are visual. I am highly auditory and kinesthetic. I am friends with a couple that is more auditory than I am and they are blind. We had a great time telling jokes and stories.

If I stared at a computer screen and did not write in longhand, I would not learn it.



 

SheilaT

(23,156 posts)
2. About ten or so years ago,
Thu Feb 26, 2015, 04:25 PM
Feb 2015

I noticed students showing up to class with laptops upon which they'd take notes. Aside from the fact that the clicking of the keys was highly annoying to me, I figured out after a bit that those who took electronic notes tended to be the poorest students in class. Now I know why.

I am a huge believer in taking notes on paper, if I want to remember something. I'm also a some-time writer of science fiction, and while I write strictly via a keyboard (typewriter in the old days, computer now), I always print out my drafts and edit manually. I just cannot figure out electronic editing, especially can't figure out how I'm supposed to transfer those electronic edits into my own electronic version of my manuscript.

Every time I see a video of some little kid who can't figure out how to turn the page in a physical book because he's only seen electronic ones, I'm not charmed, but distressed. Oh, and maybe the move to supply all school kids with tablets or computers or whatever, really isn't that good an idea after all.

Panich52

(5,829 posts)
3. I approve of tablets/computers in schools, but as supplement, not replacement
Fri Feb 27, 2015, 12:47 AM
Feb 2015

Kids need to learn electronic media these days but to sub digital f/ hard copy does a disservice. Like you, I always thought so, now know why.

We're still a textual species. Nothing beats holding a book. Or writing, not clicking out, a letter. (Remember how wonderful fresh mimeographed sheets smelled? ) BTW, your typewriter meets half the 'real world' text -- it produces an actual, physical thing.

Couple years ago I remember reading a truncated debate on teaching handwriting. That it was a question appalled me. Those funny-shaped pens and endless circle loops were a pain in 3rd grade, but I'm glad I learned. Even made a few bucks doing calligraphy many years later.

Latest Discussions»Issue Forums»Education»Why Reading and Writing o...